↓ Skip to main content

Monitor yourself! Deficient error-related brain activity predicts real-life self-control failures

Overview of attention for article published in Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, April 2018
Altmetric Badge

About this Attention Score

  • Average Attention Score compared to outputs of the same age
  • Average Attention Score compared to outputs of the same age and source

Mentioned by

twitter
5 X users

Citations

dimensions_citation
36 Dimensions

Readers on

mendeley
70 Mendeley
Title
Monitor yourself! Deficient error-related brain activity predicts real-life self-control failures
Published in
Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, April 2018
DOI 10.3758/s13415-018-0593-5
Pubmed ID
Authors

Klaus-Martin Krönke, Max Wolff, Holger Mohr, Anja Kräplin, Michael N. Smolka, Gerhard Bühringer, Thomas Goschke

Abstract

Despite their immense relevance, the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying real-life self-control failures (SCFs) are insufficiently understood. Whereas previous studies have shown that SCFs were associated with decreased activity in the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG; a region involved in cognitive control), here we consider the possibility that the reduced implementation of cognitive control in individuals with low self-control may be due to impaired performance monitoring. Following a brain-as-predictor approach, we combined experience sampling of daily SCFs with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in a Stroop task. In our sample of 118 participants, proneness to SCF was reliably predicted by low error-related activation of a performance-monitoring network (comprising anterior mid-cingulate cortex, presupplementary motor area, and anterior insula), low posterror rIFG activation, and reduced posterror slowing. Remarkably, these neural and behavioral measures predicted variability in SCFs beyond what was predicted by self-reported trait self-control. These results suggest that real-life SCFs may result from deficient performance monitoring, leading to reduced recruitment of cognitive control after responses that conflict with superordinate goals.

X Demographics

X Demographics

The data shown below were collected from the profiles of 5 X users who shared this research output. Click here to find out more about how the information was compiled.
Mendeley readers

Mendeley readers

The data shown below were compiled from readership statistics for 70 Mendeley readers of this research output. Click here to see the associated Mendeley record.

Geographical breakdown

Country Count As %
Unknown 70 100%

Demographic breakdown

Readers by professional status Count As %
Student > Bachelor 12 17%
Student > Master 9 13%
Student > Ph. D. Student 8 11%
Researcher 6 9%
Student > Doctoral Student 2 3%
Other 7 10%
Unknown 26 37%
Readers by discipline Count As %
Psychology 25 36%
Neuroscience 6 9%
Arts and Humanities 2 3%
Medicine and Dentistry 2 3%
Nursing and Health Professions 1 1%
Other 4 6%
Unknown 30 43%
Attention Score in Context

Attention Score in Context

This research output has an Altmetric Attention Score of 3. This is our high-level measure of the quality and quantity of online attention that it has received. This Attention Score, as well as the ranking and number of research outputs shown below, was calculated when the research output was last mentioned on 07 August 2018.
All research outputs
#14,459,086
of 24,671,780 outputs
Outputs from Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience
#459
of 987 outputs
Outputs of similar age
#170,202
of 332,896 outputs
Outputs of similar age from Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience
#13
of 22 outputs
Altmetric has tracked 24,671,780 research outputs across all sources so far. This one is in the 40th percentile – i.e., 40% of other outputs scored the same or lower than it.
So far Altmetric has tracked 987 research outputs from this source. They typically receive more attention than average, with a mean Attention Score of 8.5. This one has gotten more attention than average, scoring higher than 52% of its peers.
Older research outputs will score higher simply because they've had more time to accumulate mentions. To account for age we can compare this Altmetric Attention Score to the 332,896 tracked outputs that were published within six weeks on either side of this one in any source. This one is in the 47th percentile – i.e., 47% of its contemporaries scored the same or lower than it.
We're also able to compare this research output to 22 others from the same source and published within six weeks on either side of this one. This one is in the 36th percentile – i.e., 36% of its contemporaries scored the same or lower than it.